Generic placeholder image

Current Aging Science

Editor-in-Chief

ISSN (Print): 1874-6098
ISSN (Online): 1874-6128

Research Article

Role of Education and Physical Activity in Executive Function Performance of Adult Population

Author(s): Namrata Sharma* and Shweta Shenoy

Volume 16, Issue 2, 2023

Published on: 09 March, 2023

Page: [153 - 165] Pages: 13

DOI: 10.2174/1874609816666230214100557

Price: $65

Abstract

Background: Executive function performance has been extensively studied in extreme age groups, but the middle age population, where a decline could begin, is still under investigation. The role of factors like education and physical activity that might influence cognitive performance is not well understood.

Objective: The study aimed to examine the influence of age, education, and physical activity on executive function performance and the interaction effects between these factors on two subpopulations of adults, i.e., young adults and middle-aged adults.

Methods: 92 adults, 46 young adults (N = 24.4 ± 2.7 years, 25 postgraduates and 21 PhDs) and 46 middle-aged adults (N = 48.08 ± 5.79 years, 28 postgraduates and 18 PhDs), were included. Each subject performed Delayed Matching of Sample (DMS), Spatial Working Memory (SWM), and Multitasking (MTT) test on CANTAB. Physical activity levels were reported through IPAQ.

Results: Results indicated age to have a major influence on DMS, SWM, and MTT performance; however, education was only found to influence SWM. MTT and SWM performance was found to be lower in middle-aged adults in comparison to young adults. On comparison of test scores between highly active and moderately active groups of middle-aged adults, no differences were found; however, in young adults, MTT correct scores (p < 0.05) were significantly higher in the highly active group in comparison to their counterparts. Interaction between age and level of education revealed better retrieval in middle-aged adults with higher education in comparison to their counterparts.

Conclusion: In comparison to younger counterparts, executive function performance deteriorated around the middle ages. Education was found to play a protective role in executive abilities slowing during middle age. Also, physical activity may play some role in executive function performance.

Graphical Abstract

[1]
Kremen WS, Franz CE, Lyons MJ. VETSA: The vietnam era twin study of aging. Twin Res Hum Genet 2013; 16(1): 399-402.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.86] [PMID: 23110957]
[2]
Hughes ML, Agrigoroaei S, Jeon M, Bruzzese M, Lachman ME. Change in cognitive performance from midlife into old age: Findings from the midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2018; 24(8): 805-20.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000425] [PMID: 30019663]
[3]
Bielak AAM, Hughes TF, Small BJ, Dixon RA. It’s never too late to engage in lifestyle activities: Significant concurrent but not change relationships between lifestyle activities and cognitive speed. J Gerontol Ser B 2007; 62(6): P331-339.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.6.P331] [PMID: 18079417]
[4]
Salthouse TA. Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16(5): 754-60.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710000706] [PMID: 20673381]
[5]
Agrigoroaei S, Lachman ME. Cognitive functioning in midlife and old age: Combined effects of psychosocial and behavioral factors. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2011; 66B(S1): i130-40.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbr017] [PMID: 21743046]
[6]
Stern Y, Habeck C, Moeller J, et al. Brain networks associated with cognitive reserve in healthy young and old adults. Cereb Cortex 2005; 15(4): 394-402.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhh142] [PMID: 15749983]
[7]
Christensen H, MacKinnon A. The association between mental, social and physical activity and cognitive performance in young and old subjects. Age Ageing 1993; 22(3): 175-82.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ageing/22.3.175] [PMID: 8503313]
[8]
Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 2009; 30(4): 507-14.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023] [PMID: 19231028]
[9]
Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, et al. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ 2012; 344(4): d7622.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d7622] [PMID: 22223828]
[10]
Miller DI, Halpern DF. The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends Cogn Sci 2014; 18(1): 37-45.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011] [PMID: 24246136]
[11]
McCabe DP, Roediger HL III, McDaniel MA, Balota DA, Hambrick DZ. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology 2010; 24(2): 222-43.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017619] [PMID: 20230116]
[12]
Anstey KJ. Optimizing cognitive development over the life course and preventing cognitive decline: Introducing the cognitive health environment life course model (CHELM). Int J Behav Dev 2014; 38(1): 1-10.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025413512255]
[13]
Brunner EJ, Welch CA, Shipley MJ, Ahmadi-Abhari S, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M. Midlife risk factors for impaired physical and cognitive functioning at older ages: A cohort study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2017; 72(2): 237-42.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glw092] [PMID: 27271050]
[14]
Davis DHJ, Muniz-Terrera G, Keage HAD, et al. Association of delirium with cognitive decline in late life: A neuropathologic study of 3 population-based cohort studies. JAMA Psychiatry 2017; 74(3): 244-51.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3423] [PMID: 28114436]
[15]
Rönnlund M, Nilsson LG. Adult life-span patterns in WAIS-R Block Design performance: Cross-sectional versus longitudinal age gradients and relations to demographic factors. Intelligence 2006; 34(1): 63-78.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.06.004]
[16]
Zimprich D, Mascherek A. Five views of a secret: Does cognition change during middle adulthood? Eur J Ageing 2010; 7(3): 135-46.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-010-0161-5] [PMID: 28798624]
[17]
Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol 2013; 64(1): 135-68.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750] [PMID: 23020641]
[18]
Harada CN, Natelson Love MC, Triebel KL. Normal cognitive aging. Clin Geriatr Med 2013; 29(4): 737-52.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.002] [PMID: 24094294]
[19]
De Luca CR, Wood SJ, Anderson V, et al. Normative data from the CANTAB. I: Development of executive function over the lifespan. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2003; 25(2): 242-54.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.25.2.242.13639] [PMID: 12754681]
[20]
Best JR, Miller PH. A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Dev 2010; 81(6): 1641-60.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x] [PMID: 21077853]
[21]
West RL. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychol Bull 1996; 120(2): 272-92.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.272] [PMID: 8831298]
[22]
Ferguson HJ, Brunsdon VEA, Bradford EEF. The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Sci Rep 2021; 11(1): 1382.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1] [PMID: 33446798]
[23]
Ferreira D, Correia R, Nieto A, Machado A, Molina Y, Barroso J. Cognitive decline before the age of 50 can be detected with sensitive cognitive measures. Psicothema 2015; 27(3): 216-22.
[PMID: 26260927]
[24]
Cagney KA, Lauderdale DS. Education, wealth, and cognitive function in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2002; 57(2): P163-172.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/57.2.P163] [PMID: 11867664]
[25]
Lee S, Kawachi I, Berkman LF, Grodstein F. Education, other socioeconomic indicators, and cognitive function. Am J Epidemiol 2003; 157(8): 712-20.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwg042] [PMID: 12697575]
[26]
Wilson RS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Evans DA. Educational attainment and cognitive decline in old age. Neurology 2009; 72(5): 460-5.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000341782.71418.6c] [PMID: 19188578]
[27]
Foubert-Samier A, Catheline G, Amieva H, et al. Education, occupation, leisure activities, and brain reserve: A population-based study. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33(2): 423.e15-25.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.09.023] [PMID: 21074901]
[28]
Stern Y. Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2006; 20(3): S69-74.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00002093-200607001-00010] [PMID: 16917199]
[29]
Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P. Brain reserve and dementia: A systematic review. Psychol Med 2006; 36(4): 441-54.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291705006264] [PMID: 16207391]
[30]
Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, et al. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: A meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med 2010; 72(3): 239-52.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633] [PMID: 20223924]
[31]
Erickson KI, Hillman CH, Kramer AF. Physical activity, brain, and cognition. Curr Opin Behav Sci 2015; 4: 27-32.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.005]
[32]
Harvey PD. Domains of cognition and their assessment. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2019; 21(3): 227-37.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2019.21.3/pharvey] [PMID: 31749647]
[33]
Robbins TW. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB): Utility and Validation. IEE Colloquium on Computer-Aided Tests of Drug Effectiveness. 1994; pp. 3-1. IET
[34]
Wild K, Howieson D, Webbe F, Seelye A, Kaye J. Status of computerized cognitive testing in aging: A systematic review. Alzheimers Dement 2008; 4(6): 428-37.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.07.003] [PMID: 19012868]
[35]
Green R, Till C, Al-Hakeem H, et al. Assessment of neuropsychological performance in mexico city youth using the cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2019; 41(3): 246-56.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1529229] [PMID: 30336715]
[36]
Zelazo PD, Craik FIM, Booth L. Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychol 2004; 115(2-3): 167-83.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.005] [PMID: 14962399]
[37]
McNab F, Zeidman P, Rutledge RB, et al. Age-related changes in working memory and the ability to ignore distraction. Proc Natl Acad Sci 2015; 112(20): 6515-8.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1504162112] [PMID: 25941369]
[38]
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-mental state. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12(3): 189-98.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6] [PMID: 1202204]
[39]
Cambridge Cognition. CANTABeclipseTM version 3: Software user guide. Available from: https://dbglab.ru/upload/datadictionary/CANTABeclipse_Test_Administration_Guide_eng.pdf
[40]
Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, et al. A comparative study of visuospatial memory and learning in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1988; 111(3): 695-718.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.3.695] [PMID: 3382917]
[41]
Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, Robbins TW, Polkey CE, Robbins TW. Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1993; 116(5): 1159-75.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/116.5.1159] [PMID: 8221053]
[42]
Petrides M, Milner B. Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 1982; 20(3): 249-62.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(82)90100-2] [PMID: 7121793]
[43]
Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjöström M, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2003; 35(8): 1381-95.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB] [PMID: 12900694]
[44]
Wani R, Nabi S. Reliability and validity of a culturally adaptive version of the international physical activity questionnaire in indian subcontinent: A cross-sectional study. Int J Prev Med 2020; 11(1): 40.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/ijpvm.IJPVM_120_19] [PMID: 32363027]
[45]
Hartman M, Bolton E, Fehnel SE. Accounting for age differences on the wisconsin card sorting test: Decreased working memory, not inflexibility. Psychol Aging 2001; 16(3): 385-99.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.385] [PMID: 11554518]
[46]
Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology 1994; 8(4): 485-93.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485]
[47]
Kumar N, Priyadarshi B. Differential effect of aging on verbal and visuo-spatial working memory. Aging Dis 2013; 4(4): 170-7.
[PMID: 23946892]
[48]
Kane MJ, Engle RW. Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to stroop interference. J Exp Psychol Gen 2003; 132(1): 47-70.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47] [PMID: 12656297]
[49]
Milham MP, Banich MT, Webb A, et al. The relative involvement of anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in attentional control depends on nature of conflict. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2001; 12(3): 467-73.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00076-3] [PMID: 11689307]
[50]
Liu Y, Lachman ME. Education and cognition in middle age and later life: The mediating role of physical and cognitive activity. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2020; 75(7): e93-e104.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz020] [PMID: 30955036]
[51]
Kamijo K, Takeda Y. Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults. Int J Psychophysiol 2010; 75(3): 304-11.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.002] [PMID: 20079771]
[52]
Liu-Ambrose T, Nagamatsu LS, Graf P, Beattie BL, Ashe MC, Handy TC. Resistance training and executive functions: A 12-month randomized controlled trial. Arch Intern Med 2010; 170(2): 170-8.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archinternmed.2009.494] [PMID: 20101012]
[53]
Lowe CJ, Hall PA, Vincent CM, Luu K. The effects of acute aerobic activity on cognition and cross-domain transfer to eating behavior. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 267.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00267] [PMID: 24808850]
[54]
Droomers M, Schrijvers CTM, van de Mheen H, Mackenbach JP. Educational differences in leisure-time physical inactivity: A descriptive and explanatory study. Soc Sci Med 1998; 47(11): 1665-76.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00272-X] [PMID: 9877337]
[55]
Rzewnicki R, Auweele YV, Bourdeaudhuij ID. Addressing overreporting on the international physical activity questionnaire (IPAQ) telephone survey with a population sample. Public Health Nutr 2003; 6(3): 299-305.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1079/PHN2002427] [PMID: 12740079]
[56]
Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2012; 11(11): 1006-12.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6] [PMID: 23079557]
[57]
Richards M, Deary IJ. A life course approach to cognitive reserve: A model for cognitive aging and development? Ann Neurol 2005; 58(4): 617-22.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ana.20637] [PMID: 16178025]
[58]
Gómez-Pinilla F. Brain foods: The effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9(7): 568-78.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2421] [PMID: 18568016]
[59]
Clare L, Wu YT, Teale JC, et al. Potentially modifiable lifestyle factors, cognitive reserve, and cognitive function in later life: A cross-sectional study. PLoS Med 2017; 14(3): e1002259.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002259] [PMID: 28323829]
[60]
Cheng ST. Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: The role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2016; 18(9): 85.
[http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2] [PMID: 27481112]

Rights & Permissions Print Cite
© 2024 Bentham Science Publishers | Privacy Policy